- Research
- Open access
- Published:
Cognitive function in older patients and their stress challenge using different anesthesia regimes: a single center observational study
BMC Anesthesiology volume 23, Article number: 6 (2023)
Abstract
Background
With increasing age older patients are at higher risk for cognitive decline after surgery. Even tailored anesthesia procedures in older patients remain a high risk for postoperative cognitive disorder. Additional stress derived from anxiety and anesthesia itself can negatively impact postoperative cognitive outcomes. The objective of this study was to evaluate the impact of general versus regional anesthesia on postoperative cognitive disorder and indicators of perioperative stress in elderly undergoing surgery.
Methods
In this single center prospective study between December 2014 and November 2015, 46 patients aged 50 to 85 years undergoing dermatology surgery were enrolled. Patients were stratified by receiving general versus regional nerve anesthesia. On three consecutive days, saliva cortisol levels were analyzed three times per day. Cognitive function was assessed on the day before and the day after surgery using comprehensive neuropsychological testing of multiple cognitive functions including memory, executive function, attention and processing speed.
Results
Comparing the regional anesthesia group (RAG, n = 28) with the general anesthesia group (GAG, n = 18) no significant difference in the postoperative cognitive function was observed. However, patients in the GAG had significantly higher postoperative cortisol levels when compared to patients in the RAG. In both groups, a peak of cortisol value was detected on the day of surgery, which was higher in the GAG in comparison to the RAG.
Conclusions
We did not observe a difference in postoperative cognitive function between patients undergoing regional or general anesthesia for dermatology surgery. However, we found lower cortisol level in the RAG. Based on these findings, future studies should investigate alternatives to reduce stress in a general anesthesia setting.
Trial registration
ClinicalTrials.gov ID: NCT02505815.
Background
With an ageing society, health care providers worldwide face similar difficulties encountering older patients. Besides multi-organ dysfunction, older patients are at higher risk for cognitive decline, challenging the medical and surgical treatment of this ageing population. Cognition requires numerous components of cortical and subcortical domains like memory, attention and executive function in order to identify, perceive and solve problems and to generate future knowledge and experience [1]. Therefore, the cognitive integrity of older patients is of pivotal importance in everyday life for the activities of daily living (ADL) and quality of life (QOL). Anesthesia procedures in older patients pose a high risk for postoperative cognitive dysfunction regardless of their current preserved cognitive function [2, 3]. In elderly patients, these symptoms are detected more frequently compared to younger patients after general anesthesia [4]. Moreover, the diminished cognitive flexibility persists longer and in its severe occurrence can lead to permanent postoperative cognitive disorder (POCD). Anxiety is a well-known phenomenon in elderly surgical patients and has a pivotal impact on perioperative stress. Additional stress derived from anesthesia influences postoperative physical and cognitive outcomes [5], leading to an increase in POCD and impairment in elderly surgical patients with an incidence of over 45% [6, 7]. Thus, accurately planned anesthesia procedures could help to reduce perceived stress and might therefore attenuate any negative impact on cognitive postoperative function. Risk factors for the development of POCD include age, low educational level and preexisting reduced cognitive reserve [2, 3]. Moreover, the anesthesia procedure can play an important role, but current understanding of what technique carries the lowest risk is unknown. Some studies suggested that procedures with regional are more recommended in comparison to general anesthesia to avoid POCD. However, this could not be proven yet [8]. The combination of elevated stress level and regional anesthesia are believed to increase POCD in elderly patients. This observation leads to the question if a general anesthesia is associated with lower stress level and consequently translates into a reduced risk of POCD. In the present study, we investigated the association between general versus regional anesthesia in patients undergoing dermatology surgery and postoperative cognitive disorder. We further investigated whether a general versus regional anesthesia was associated with post-procedural cortisol levels.
Methods
In this single-center observational study, two patient arms were compared; the general anesthesia group (GAG) vs. the regional anesthesia group (RAG). This study was conducted at the University Hospital of Erlangen, Germany between December 2014 and November 2015 in accordance with the guidelines for Good Clinical Practice and the Declaration of Helsinki. The study approval was given by the local Ethics committee (Ethikkommission der Medizinischen Fakultät der Friedrich-Alexander-Universität Erlangen-Nürnberg, Erlangen, Germany) (reference number: 245_14 B, on 18.09.2014) (ClinicalTrials.gov ID: NCT02505815). Each patient read and signed a consent form before included in the study.
Patients
After written informed consent, 48 patients were enrolled to the study. Inclusion criteria were age between 50 and 85 years, an American Society of Anesthesiologist (ASA) physical status less than IV as well as an estimated postoperative stay of at least two days after receiving either regional or general anesthesia. Patients with a history of neurological or neuropsychological deficits or diseases including Cushing’s disease, stroke or epilepsy, or with prescriptions of neuropsychological medication were excluded from the study. Moreover, patients with alcohol and illicit drug abuse, time shifts within the test protocol and patients undergoing cardiac surgery were not included into the study. Moreover, patients with perioperative medication like benzodiazepine treatment and any glucocorticoids were not included into the study. Patients susceptible to postoperative nausea and vomiting (PONV) were excluded from this study. In view of the fact that a maximum of three patients can be tested sensibly every day so that the test phases took place in the same period and do not take place in the evening hours, the respective groups were gradually completed in one after the other. However, as a result, more patients were asked than were ultimately included in the study. To avoid a systematic bias all patients have been enrolled in the department of dermatology for a comparable duration and invasiveness of surgery. Malignant neoplasms of the skin and subcutis were excised at different parts of the body. These include melanomas, basalcell carcinomas and diseased skin and subcutaneous tissue with as yet uncertain pathology, which were mainly found on the scalp and facial skin. The primary disease had no influence or effects on the cortisol parameters or the cognitive function of all the study patients. Moreover, the examiners were blinded to the scheduled form of anesthesia.
Cortisol
In order to quantify a biomarker for the individual stress level, patients submitted a saliva sample at three specified times of the day on the day prior to surgery, then on the day of surgery and first postoperative day. Prior to salvia collection, patients were instructed to not brush their teeth, rinse their mouth, smoke or eat. The cortisol daily profile was determined from these saliva samples. For this purpose, in the morning immediately after waking up, at noon and in the evening, swabs (Salivette→ Sarstedt, Nümbrecht, Germany) were moistened with saliva by means of chewing. Patients were instructed to adhere to the same time of day with the saliva collection. On the day of surgery, the second probe was collected immediately before surgery and postoperatively after transfer from the postoperative care facility at a similar time as the other day before. Saliva samples were temporarily stored in the refrigerator at 2–4° Celsius and then transferred to be stored at -80° Celsius until analysis. Cortisol concentrations were determined by liquid chromatography tandem mass spectrometry as previously described at the University Hospital of Erlangen [9,10,11].
Cognitive assessment
In order to assess potential alterations in patients’ cognitive function, a cognitive assessment was carried out at two different time points using a neuropsychological test assessment [12, 13]. The baseline testing was recorded on the day before surgery and the second survey on the first postoperative day. To ensure comparability, the schedule of both assessments was synchronized. Furthermore, numerous test systems are used in the literature, so that comparability is still difficult. We aimed to evaluate as many different cognitive domains as possible with our assessment. To avoid learning effects, only instruments with available parallel versions were applied. To avoid fatigue, test time was limited to a maximum half an hour. The neuropsychological test battery was composed as follows:
We used the DemTect test as psychomotor screening test to detect mild cognitive functional impairments [14]. The DemTect test assess “verbal memory”, “verbal fluency”, “cognitive flexibility”, and “attention” in five consecutive, separate tasks while accounting for age and education. To evaluate the global memory function we used the Rivermead Behavioral Memory test (RBMT) [15]. To assess the patient’s short term memory capacity we used the digit span forward test and its backwards variant the digit span backward test for verbal working memory (digit span backward test) [16]. The cognitive domains of attention, concentration and executive function were addressed with the Stroop test [17]. Finally, due to its nine-subtest structure, memory and attention deficits were by the SKT test [18]. Further details have been previously described [12, 13]. Table 1 summarizes cognitive assessments and corresponding tested cognitive function and domain.
Clinical Protocol
After participants were included in the study, their medication plans were documented and it was ensured that no chronic medication was prescribed that had a modulating effect on neurocognitive function. At the same time, questions were asked about previous experiences with PONV, since no corticoids may be administered in the study protocol. Thus, PONV susceptible patients were not included in the study. For premedication, benzodiazepines were strictly avoided in all patients and instead, if necessary, clonidine 75 or 150 µg was given preoperatively. Total intravenous anesthesia with Remifentanil and Propofol was chosen in the GAG and managed as target controlled infusion (TCI). Before placing a laryngeal mask, the patients received 0.1 mg Fentanyl for anesthesia induction and additionally weight-adapted Rocuronium was administrated for endotracheal intubation as a neuromuscular blocking agent. During the entire surgery, vital data were monitored and in particular attention was paid to perioperative hemodynamic stability in order to avoid hypotensive phases. We defined a more than 20% decrease from preoperative baseline awake systolic blood pressure measurement as a hypotensive phase. Moreover, hypotension defined as systolic blood pressure lower than 80 mmHg was avoided by administration of 1-2-ml Theodrenalin-Cafedrin. In addition, patients’ peripheral oxygen saturation was monitored. Patients were pre-oxygenated with 100% oxygen before induction of anesthesia until the end-tidal oxygen concentration reached values higher than 80%, and patients received 100% oxygen during emergence before extubation, likewise. After extubation, patients were transferred to recovery room. Oxygen was given during the transfer and was continued in the recovery room as to institutional standards. In the RAG, patients received local anesthetic for the surgery area with Lidocaine 1% s.c. Vital data were also monitored in this group and oxygen was insufflated by simple face mask or nasal cannula. These patients were also transferred to the recovery room for postoperative monitoring. Both groups received 7.5 mg to 15 mg piritramide over 20 min depending on total body weight for pain therapy, if pain severity of 4 points or more on an 11-point numerical pain scale was evaluated.
Data management and statistical analysis
Statistical analysis was performed using IBM SPSS Statistics version 27.0 for macOS platforms. Outliers were identified using the Grubbs test and were not included in further data analysis. To test normality for scale variables, the Kolmogorov-Smirnov test and the Shapiro-Wilks test were used. The Mann-Whitney U test was used to compare the mean scores of both groups based on age and cortisol levels. Spearman’s test was used to analyze the correlation between age and cortisol levels. Fisher’s exact test was used to compare the differences in risk factors between the two groups and a univariate ANOVA was performed to analyze the influence of risk factors on cortisol levels.
Comparing the differences in cognitive tests between the two groups, the Mann-Whitney U test was used. Due to the various tests, the level of significance was set at p < 0.005 for the two-factor variance-analysis, whereas it was defined at p < 0.05 for all other statistical tests.
Results
We screened 67 patients for eligibility and had to exclude 12 patients from participating in the study because of PONV, 4 patients due to neuropsychological medication and 5 patients regarding individual personal reasons. Overall, 48 patients were included in the study, out of which 2 patients did not complete the test phase. Reasons for discontinuing the study varied in nature, such as discomfort after surgery or pain, or missing cortisone samples (Fig. 1)
Demographic and clinical data are given in Table 2.
The mean age in the RAG was 75.4 years. In the GAG the mean value was 62.0 years. In the RAG 17 out of 28 patients were male, whereas 15 out of 18 patients were male in the GAG, with no statistically significant difference (p = 0.095). Level of significance (*p = 0.05). However, the ASA value was significantly different between both groups, with a higher value in the RAG. Moreover, the duration of anesthesia was longer and nicotine abuse was higher in the RAG.
Comparison cortisol level in GAG and RAG
Cortisol levels were determined on three consecutive days at three fixed timepoints. Individual cortisol values out of the complete set of cortisol measurement on a single day were missing for 17 patients. This single missing cortisol measurement for each of these patients occurred completely randomly and distributed throughout the study. Therefore, the entire cohort was analyzed in a first step, and the same analysis was carried excluding the patients with missing values in a second step. A comparison of the mean values in this subgroup analysis showed the same result with a statistically significant difference in each case. All cortisol values from both groups are presented in Table 3.
The mean value of the total cortisol level was 5.47 U/L in the GAG and 2.56 U/L in the RAG (n = 46, p = 0.006). The mean value in the subgroup (n = 29) of the total cortisol level was 6.36 U/L in the GAG and 2.35 U/L in the RAG (p = 0.028).
Comparison cortisol level on individual days
The mean cortisol values were consistently higher on all three days in patients receiving general anesthesia compared to patients receiving regional anesthesia (Table 3). The analysis of the entire cohort revealed similar results as after excluding patients with at least one missing cortisol value one any of the three days. Moreover, a mixed ANOVA revealed similar pattern cortisol values on any of the three days over all patients.
The cortisol values and the analysis of the subgroup (n = 29) excluding individuals with a missing value on any of the three days can be found in Table 3. This analysis revealed a similar pattern as the analysis in all patients.
Correlation of age and cortisol level
There was no correlation between age and total cortisol level in either group (GAG: p = 0.78 and RAG: p = 0.422).
Distribution of risk factors for cortisol release perioperatively and selected chronic medication
We analyzed how often a risk factor occurred in the respective group and whether this difference was statistically significant. The risk factors frequencies are related to the respective group and the check for statistical significance are listed in Table 2. There were more patients with arterial hypertension and nicotine abuse in the RAG. We analyzed how often a certain chronic medication was taken by the patient. The distribution of the frequency and the respective difference between the two groups is shown in Table 2.
There was a statistically significant difference observed in the RAG group for higher proportion of patients taking AT III antagonists and more diuretics than in the GAG. Moreover, the age was significantly higher in the RAG in comparison to GAG.
Influence of risk factors and chronic medication on cortisol level
In exploratory analyses, we investigated the association between individual risk factors of POCD and cortisol levels. There was no association between a history of arterial hypertension, nicotine abuse, the use of AT III antagonists or diuretics and POCD (Table 4).
Differences in cognitive tests pre- and postoperatively and comparison of both groups
Each test of the neurocognitive assessment was analyzed preoperatively and postoperatively. When comparing both groups, the FWIT test showed a better result in the RAG group for both assessment phases. The RMBT test showed a better result preoperatively in the GAG. The SKT memory test and the Digit Span backwards test showed a better result in the GAG postoperatively.
However, between the two groups the differences between the pre- and postoperative results in the neurocognitive testing showed no significant difference in any of the tests. By comparing the test result differences, itself, we elicit only in one test system a significant difference (see Table 5). However, the digital span test forward and digital span test backwards itself were not significantly different. Thus, only if both tests are analyzed together, there is a significant difference (see Table 6).
After finding a significant difference in patient age between the two groups, we performed an age matched subgroup analysis using a range of three years of age around the calculated mean value. Results are presented in Table 7 and demonstrate significant differences in anesthesia time and nicotine abuse. However, these statistical differences have been demonstrated in the entire cohort before. Moreover, we consecutively performed a subgroup analysis for the parameter of height, weight, ASA physical status and duration of anesthesia. There was no statistically significant result within these subgroups.
Regarding the cortisol level the subgroup analysis elicit no significant differences between both groups. However, the cortisol level in the RAG were tendentially still lower than in the GAG (see Table 8).
Moreover, we performed a subgroup analysis regarding the cognitive assessment results. However, only in a single test section we found a significant difference between both groups (see Table 9).
Discussion
The aim of this study was to evaluate the association of different anesthesia strategies (regional versus general anesthesia) with postoperative stress, indicated by cortisol levels and cognitive outcome in elderly patients. When comparing the two study groups we did not find any differences in the development of early postoperative cognitive disorder. Moreover, there was no significant difference in the baseline testing values when comparing age matched pairs.
However, we observed that patients receiving general anesthesia had higher postoperative cortisol levels at each of the scheduled days. It is also noticeable that in both groups we found a peak of cortisol value on the day of surgery, which was more pronounced in the GAG in comparison to the RAG.
In addition, the average patient age was significantly higher in the regional anesthesia group. We assume that due to a higher patient age as well as an expected shorter duration of surgery these patients have been more likely been planned for a regional anesthesia procedure. Likewise, the advanced age might have tempted the anesthesiologist to classify these patients rather into the RAG group. This assumption can also be assumed if the ASA values of the patients are considered. In the regional anesthesia group the mean ASA value was significantly higher. In considering the risk of anesthesia and the constitution of the patient in terms of ASA quantification, those who were presumably more seriously ill were more likely to receive regional anesthesia. However, we performed a subgroup analysis in an age matched cohort. Even in this subgroup we could not demonstrate a significant difference between the two subgroups either in the cortisol evaluation or in the cognitive assessment.
Our cognitive assessment examined functions that also included executive functions which belong to higher cognitive processes [19]. With increasing age, a decline in the efficiency of executive functions in healthy older people compared to younger people has been reported, along with acute stress aggravates age-related cognitive deficits [20,21,22]. Some authors hypothesized that higher memory functions with complex cognitive patterns, such as executive functions, are supplied with less energy in an acute stress situation [23]. Furthermore, other studies were able to demonstrate that acute stress partly worsens executive functions [24, 25]. These accompanying impairments can be attributed to a stress-related increased release of catecholamines and glucocorticoids [26]. However, based on our data, although patients in the GA group had higher postoperative cortisol levels, this was not associated with increased POCD.
We assumed that patients scheduled for surgery experience stress in particular. Especially those who are also awake while undergoing surgical procedure and are given local anesthesia, experience additional stressor of an unfamiliar environment. Kazmierski and colleagues were able to demonstrate that after coronary artery bypass graft surgery, increased cortisol levels can lead to cognitive dysfunction [27]. In a non-cardiac-surgical study it was shown that regional anesthesia leads to better neurocognitive test scores than general anesthesia in patients undergoing total knee arthroplasty [28]. In this study the cognitive test values in the regional anesthesia group were unchanged and in the general anesthesia group on the seventh postoperative day cognitive test values were significantly reduced [28]. In our data, the cognitive values in the group undergoing regional anesthesia are also unchanged. However, these findings also apply to the general anesthesia group. This could be explained by the different time of assessment when the cognitive testing was carried out. Our study scheduled the cognitive assessment on the first postoperative day, whereas in the comparative study, the assessment was scheduled on the seventh day.
Although some studies suggested that procedures under regional anesthesia are more suitable in comparison to general anesthesia to avoid POCD, this could not be proven [8]. Thus, available literature has conflicting results regarding anesthesia approaches and the risk of POCD. This could be due to the different surgeries, duration of surgery and anesthesia, a varying patient population and ASA value or different, individual stressors. Rasmussen and co-workers could previously not show any significant differences between regional and general anesthesia with regard to POCD in concordance with our observation [29]. However, Mandal and co-authors showed a significantly reduced cognitive function after general anesthesia compared to a regional anesthesia procedure [30]. A more recent comparative study showed a reduced cognitive function in all study patients, though the magnitude and duration of the cognitive impairment were less pronounced in the patients with epidural anesthesia than in the general anesthesia group [31].
Yuan and co-workers associated an increased risk of cognitive dysfunction to older patients who had elevated salivary cortisol levels preoperatively. They assumed an association between a preoperative neuroendocrine disorder and POCD [32]. The authors also believed that short-term, stress-induced cortisol release could be adapted, whereas chronic stress provokes higher levels of glucocorticoids and could even cause structural brain changes, which leads to cognitive impairment in neurodegenerative diseases [33, 34]. By this background, Mu and colleagues also reported increased cortisol levels on the first postoperative morning to be associated with an increased risk of early postoperative POCD [35]. In addition, it is assumed that an increased postoperative cortisol level leads to a higher risk for the development of delirium [36]. However, this correlation of increased cortisol levels and POCD as well as delirium could not be proven by Glumac and colleagues in a cohort study of cardiosurgical patients according to a strict POCD definition [37]. Likewise, there were no significant differences in the cortisol values in the patients at the sixth postoperative day between the POCD and non-POCD patients after cardiac surgery and general anesthesia. In contrast, in a group of patients of the same age with hip fracture surgery and spinal anesthesia, the baseline plasma cortisol level was the same. However, the POCD patients plasma cortisol levels were significantly increased postoperatively [38]. Moreover, the authors reported that older age and a lower educational level might also be suspected for the occurrence of POCD. In the present study, we also found consistently lower cortisol levels in the regional anesthesia group compared to the general anesthesia group. However, the comparatively higher cortisol levels in the general anesthesia group were not associated with a postoperative cognitive deficit in our study. Even if the subgroup analysis is considered with age matched pairs, the cortisol levels in the GAG were above those in the RAG, but were no longer significantly different and did not show any postoperative cognitive disorders. Furthermore, the only brief increase in cortisol levels observed in the entire general anesthesia group did not lead to any changes in cognitive function. This can possibly be explained by compensatory mechanisms of patients themselves [33]. Additionally, increased cortisol values after stress have been expected in elderly patients [39]. It is conceivable that this elevated cortisol level, which only occurs on the day of the surgery in the general anesthesia group, is accompanied by a stress reaction resulting from fear of the anesthesia. It was shown that the fear of general anesthesia is significantly greater than that of regional anesthesia [40]. However, it was already documented years ago that fear of anesthesia decreases with increasing age [41]. Therefore, this might explain our results of low cortisol levels in regional anesthesia without a peak.
In the current study, we aimed to investigate the influence of regional or general anesthesia and the accompanying stress for the patient with regard to the development of POCD. We used an assessment battery for cognitive evaluation and at the same time determined salivary cortisol on three perioperative days. It was found that the cortisol levels in the regional anesthesia group were lower than in the general anesthesia group. The demographic influencing variables and patients risk factors had no influence on the cortisol level. Although the cortisol levels in the general anesthesia group were significantly higher on the day of surgery in comparison to the regional anesthesia group, this had no influence on the cognitive outcome results. In the cognitive assessment, no differences were found between both groups regarding the difference between pre- and postoperative outcome.
Limitations
There are some considerable study limitations to the interpretation of these findings. First, average age in our study was 62 years in the GAG and 75 years in the RAG, respectively, which may be still too young to elicit a significant age-related changes in executive function respectively cognitive dysfunction [21]. Secondly, missing cortisol values could also have an alternating effect in the statistics. Missing study data in our data set can be further attributed to insufficient moisture penetration of the cortisol cotton wool. These individually missing cortisol measurements were completely randomly distributed. Thus, no systematic bias can be assumed. Future studies should include larger study population in order to identify small effects of altered cognitive performance. On the other hand, another limitation could arise from the cognitive tests themselves. The cognitive assessment used in the study follows a conservative neuropsychological examination, which is subject to a certain laboratory-like quality such as reliability and validity. However, these assessments cannot be carried out otherwise under standardized conditions. The conditions are therefore also the same for each test person. Nevertheless, it would therefore be conceivable that the influence of stress on cognitive performance could not be detected under standardized assessment conditions. In the light of these considerations, the effects of stress on cognitive performance in everyday life have not been investigated and thus escapes study evaluation. Furthermore, from a cognitive point of view, healthy subjects could not have been challenged sufficiently with the cognitive assessment. Even though the length of the individual tests should not be underestimated, the order of the tests in the second repetition part should possibly have been rearranged in order to prevent a possible habituation effect. Nevertheless, we have used two different individual test versions and limited the test duration in favor of a preserving concentration and in order to avoid a fatigue bias. Schwabe and Wolf demonstrated that an acute stress reaction can affect certain cognitive domains for up to 90 min (e.g. memory retrieval impairment) [42]. However, to what extent this effect can still be demonstrated on the first postoperative day will have to be demonstrated by future studies. This also includes detailed studies on general timing effects and their influence on a surgical stress reaction or the neurobiological effect of pure cortisol secretion on cognitive performance.
Conclusions
In conclusion, we could not detect any differences in postoperative cognitive outcome between patients receiving regional versus general anesthesia. However, we found a lower cortisol level in the regional anesthesia group. Based on these findings, future studies might further elucidate the impact of different anesthesia regimens on perioperative stress, and its impact on patient outcome.
Availability data and materials
The datasets generated and analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.
Abbreviations
- ACE:
-
Angiotensin-convertingenzyme
- ADL:
-
activities of daily living
- ASA:
-
American Society of Anesthesiologists
- FWIT:
-
Color-Word-Interference-Test
- GAG:
-
general anesthesia group
- POCD:
-
postoperative cognitive disorder
- PONV:
-
postoperative nausea and vomiting
- QOL:
-
quality of life
- RAG:
-
regional anesthesia group
- RBMT:
-
Rivermead Behavioral Memory Test
- SKT:
-
Short Cognitive Performance Test
- TCI:
-
Target Controlled Infusion
References
Hanning CD. Postoperative cognitive dysfunction. Br J Anaesth. 2005;95:82–7.
Tse L, Bowering JB, Schwarz SKW, Moore RL, Sztramko R, Barr AM. Incidence and risk factors for impaired mobility in older cardiac surgery patients during the early postoperative period: impaired mobility in cardiac surgery patients. Geriatr Gerontol Int. 2015;15:276–81.
Lee Y-S, Kim Y-B, Lee S-H, Park Y-S, Park S-W. The prevalence of undiagnosed presurgical cognitive impairment and its postsurgical clinical impact in older patients undergoing lumbar spine surgery. J Korean Neurosurg Soc. 2016;59:287.
Cottrell JE, Hartung J. Anesthesia and cognitive outcome in Elderly Patients: a narrative viewpoint. J Neurosurg Anesthesiol. 2020;32:9–17.
McIsaac DI, MacDonald DB, Aucoin SD. Frailty for Perioperative Clinicians: a narrative review. Anesth Analgesia. 2020;130:1450–60.
Martina JA, Diab HI, Brady OA, Puertollano R. TFEB and TFE 3 are novel components of the integrated stress response. EMBO J. 2016;35:479–95.
Sherman JB, Chatterjee A, Urman RD, Culley DJ, Crosby GJ, Cooper Z, Javedan H, Hepner DL, Bader AM. Implementation of Routine Cognitive Screening in the Preoperative Assessment Clinic. A A Pract. 2019;12(4):125–7.
Zywiel MG, Prabhu A, Perruccio AV, Gandhi R. The influence of Anesthesia and Pain Management on Cognitive Dysfunction after Joint Arthroplasty: a systematic review. Clin Orthop Relat Res. 2014;472:1453–66.
Koal T, Schmiederer D, Pham-Tuan H, Röhring C, Rauh M. Standardized LC–MS/MS based steroid hormone profile-analysis. J Steroid Biochem Mol Biol. 2012;129:129–38.
Rauh M, Gröschl M, Rascher W, Dörr HG. Automated, fast and sensitive quantification of 17α-hydroxy-progesterone, androstenedione and testosterone by tandem mass spectrometry with on-line extraction. Steroids. 2006;71:450–8.
Fahlbusch FB, Ruebner M, Rascher W, Rauh M. Combined quantification of corticotropin-releasing hormone, cortisol-to-cortisone ratio and progesterone by liquid chromatography–Tandem mass spectrometry in placental tissue. Steroids. 2013;78:888–95.
Wagner S, Quente J, Staedtler S, Koch K, Richter-Schmidinger T, Kornhuber J, et al. A high risk of sleep apnea is associated with less postoperative cognitive dysfunction after intravenous anesthesia: results of an observational pilot study. BMC Anesthesiol. 2018;18:139.
Wagner S, Sutter L, Wagenblast F, Walther A, Schiff J-H. Short term cognitive function after sevoflurane anesthesia in patients suspect to obstructive sleep apnea syndrome: an observational study. BMC Anesthesiol. 2021;21:150.
Kalbe E, Kessler J, Calabrese P, Smith R, Passmore AP, Brand M, et al. DemTect: a new, sensitive cognitive screening test to support the diagnosis of mild cognitive impairment and early dementia. Int J Geriat Psychiatry. 2004;19:136–43.
Bolló-Gasol S, Piñol-Ripoll G, Cejudo-Bolivar JC, Llorente-Vizcaino A, Peraita-Adrados H. Evaluación ecológica en el deterioro cognitivo leve y enfermedad de Alzheimer mediante el Rivermead behavioural memory test. Neurología. 2014;29:339–45.
Cheng Y, Wang YJ, Yan JC, Zhou R, Zhou HD. Effects of carotid artery stenting on cognitive function in patients with mild cognitive impairment and carotid stenosis. Experimental and Therapeutic Medicine. 2013;5:1019–24.
Dittrich WH, Johansen T. Cognitive deficits of executive functions and decision-making in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Scand J Psychol. 2013;54:393–400.
Stemmler M, Petermann F, Daseking M, Siebert J, Schott H, Lehfeld H, et al. Diagnostik und Verlauf von kognitiven Fähigkeiten bei älteren Menschen. Gesundheitswesen. 2013;75:761–7.
Cabeza R. Hemispheric asymmetry reduction in older adults: the HAROLD model. Psychol Aging. 2002;17:85–100.
Podell JE, Sambataro F, Murty VP, Emery MR, Tong Y, Das S, et al. Neurophysiological correlates of age-related changes in working memory updating. NeuroImage. 2012;62:2151–60.
Bryan J, Luszcz MA. Measurement of executive function: considerations for detecting adult age differences. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2000;22:40–55.
Buckner RL. Memory and executive function in aging and AD. Neuron. 2004;44:195–208.
de Kloet ER, Joëls M, Holsboer F. Stress and the brain: from adaptation to disease. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2005;6:463–75.
Butts KA, Weinberg J, Young AH, Phillips AG. Glucocorticoid receptors in the prefrontal cortex regulate stress-evoked dopamine efflux and aspects of executive function. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2011;108:18459–64.
Starcke K, Wiesen C, Trotzke P, Brand M. Effects of Acute Laboratory Stress on Executive Functions. Front Psychol. 2016;7:461.
Arnsten AFT. Stress signalling pathways that impair prefrontal cortex structure and function. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2009;10:410–22.
Kazmierski J, Banys A, Latek J, Bourke J, Jaszewski R. Cortisol levels and neuropsychiatric diagnosis as markers of postoperative delirium: a prospective cohort study. Crit Care. 2013;17:R38.
Edipoglu IS, Celik F. The Associations between cognitive dysfunction, stress biomarkers, and administered anesthesia type in total knee arthroplasties: prospective, randomized trial. Pain Physician. 2019;22:495–507.
Rasmussen LS, Christiansen M, Rasmussen H, Kristensen PA, Moller JT. Do blood concentrations of neurone specific enolase and S-100 beta protein reflect cognitive dysfunction after abdominal surgery?ISPOCD Group. Br J Anaesth. 2000;84:242–4.
Pal R, Kar S, Kundu K, Sarkar U, Gupta S, Mandal S, et al. Impact of general versus epidural anesthesia on early post-operative cognitive dysfunction following hip and knee surgery. J Emerg Trauma Shock. 2011;4:23.
Ezhevskaya AA, Ovechkin AM, Prusakova ZB, Zagrekov VI, Mlyavykh SG, Anderson DG. Relationship among anesthesia technique, surgical stress, and cognitive dysfunction following spinal surgery: a randomized trial. J Neurosurgery: Spine. 2019;31:894–901.
Han Y, Han L, Dong M-M, Sun Q-C, Zhang Z-F, Ding K, et al. Preoperative salivary cortisol AM/PM ratio predicts early postoperative cognitive dysfunction after noncardiac surgery in Elderly Patients. Anesth Analgesia. 2019;128:349–57.
Carroll D, Ginty AT, Whittaker AC, Lovallo WR, de Rooij SR. The behavioural, cognitive, and neural corollaries of blunted cardiovascular and cortisol reactions to acute psychological stress. Neurosci Biobehavioral Reviews. 2017;77:74–86.
McEwen BS, Bowles NP, Gray JD, Hill MN, Hunter RG, Karatsoreos IN, et al. Mechanisms of stress in the brain. Nat Neurosci. 2015;18:1353–63.
Mu D-L, Li L-H, Wang D-X, Li N, Shan G-J, Li J, et al. High postoperative serum cortisol level is Associated with increased risk of cognitive dysfunction early after coronary artery bypass graft surgery: a prospective cohort study. PLoS ONE. 2013;8:e77637.
Mu D-L, Wang D-X, Li L-H, Shan G-J, Li J, Yu Q-J, et al. High serum cortisol level is associated with increased risk of delirium after coronary artery bypass graft surgery: a prospective cohort study. Crit Care. 2010;14:R238.
Glumac S, Kardum G, Karanović N. A prospective cohort evaluation of the Cortisol response to Cardiac surgery with occurrence of early postoperative cognitive decline. Med Sci Monit. 2018;24:977–86.
Ji M-H, Shen J-C, Gao R, Liu X-Y, Yuan H-M, Dong L, et al. Early postoperative cognitive dysfunction is associated with higher cortisol levels in aged patients following hip fracture surgery. J Anesth. 2013;27:942–4.
Chrousos GP, Kino T. Glucocorticoid action networks and complex psychiatric and/or somatic disorders. Stress. 2007;10:213–9.
Celik F, Edipoglu IS. Evaluation of preoperative anxiety and fear of anesthesia using APAIS score. Eur J Med Res. 2018;23:41.
Shevde K, Panagopoulos G. A survey of 800 patients' knowledge, attitudes, and concerns regarding anesthesia. Anesth Analg. 1991;73(2):190–8.
Schwabe L, Wolf OT. Timing matters: temporal dynamics of stress effects on memory retrieval. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2014;14:1041–8.
Acknowledgements
The authors thank all the patients, who were interviewed and assessed. They also thank all staff members at the recruiting center as well as Mareike Kirsch and Stefanie Rockstroh, in particular.
Funding
Open Access funding enabled and organized by Projekt DEAL.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
SW helped to design and to conduct the study, to analyze the data and write the manuscript. MB, EA, HM, OK, CAFvA, CT and AW helped to analyze the data and write the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Ethics approval and consent to participate
This study was conducted in accordance with the guidelines for Good Clinical Practice and the Declaration of Helsinki due to ethical principles for medical research involving human subjects. The study approval was given by the local Ethics committee (Ethikkommission der Medizinischen Fakultät der Friedrich-Alexander-Universität Erlangen-Nürnberg, Erlangen, Germany) (reference number: 245_14 B, on 18.09.2014) (ClinicalTrials.gov ID: NCT02505815). Each patient read and signed a consent form before enclosed in the study. Informed consent was obtained from all participants.
Consent to publish
Not applicable.
Competing interests
CAFvA received honoraria from serving on the scientific advisory board of Biogen, Roche, Novo Nordisk, MindAhead UG and Dr. Willmar Schwabe GmbH and has received funding for travel and speaker honoraria from Biogen, Roche diagnostics AG, Medical Tribune Verlagsgesellschaft mbH, Landesvereinigung für Gesundheit und Akademie für Sozialmedizin Niedersachsen e. V. and Dr. Willmar Schwabe GmbH and has received research support from Roche diagnostics AG and research funding from the Innovationsfond (Fund of the Federal Joint Committee, Gemeinsamer Bundesausschuss, G-BA Grants No. VF1_2016 − 201; 01NVF21010; 01VSF21019).
The other authors declare that they have no conflicts of interest regarding the content of this article.
Additional information
Publisher’s Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.
About this article
Cite this article
Wagner, S., Breitkopf, M., Ahrens, E. et al. Cognitive function in older patients and their stress challenge using different anesthesia regimes: a single center observational study. BMC Anesthesiol 23, 6 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12871-022-01960-7
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12871-022-01960-7